Int J Appl Sports Sci > Volume 31(1); 2019 > Article
Zhu and Han: Effects of different sport participations on prosocial and antisocial behaviors

Abstract

People might mythically believe sport affecting on social behavior positively, and that an intensive participation could provide better social behavior to youth and society. In addition, some people have propagandized the myth to promote the positive utilization of sport. However, the literature has not provided an agreed consensus as to sport participation increasing prosocial behaviors and reducing antisocial behaviors. Given that, the present study attempted to examine the conflicting issue between the literature. Therefore, the purpose of this study is to examine the effect of sport participation on prosocial and antisocial behavior in the comparison between the elite and the recreational. To achieve the purpose of this study, survey questionnaires were distributed, and 589 responses were analyzed using a factors analysis, a one-way MANOVA.
The results were as followed. First, compared to most of the literature not specifying factors of prosocial and antisocial behaviors, the present study identified social behavior factors using an existing questionnaire. Second, there were differences in social behaviors between the elite sport participant and the recreational sport participant. Third, the recreational sport participation and the elite sport participation differ on prosocial behavior especially in high leadership, social facilitation, and high group cohesion. Fourth, the elite sport participant and the recreational sport participant have no significant difference on antisocial behaviors.

Introduction

In recent years, social development through sport is a highly topical issue. At the mention of sport, there is a significant amount of news and reports support that sport is an effective instrument that can help improve the quality and development of human, families and countries in general. There are also many sports organizations strongly promoted the sport as a tool for social change. The UN (2008) recognizes that sports are incredibly valuable for improving social development and economic progress. The International Olympic Committee (IOC) has a project, which uses sport as a tool for development and advances the Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs). Many academics have conducted many studies and they have come to the view that sport has both positive and negative effects on social development.
The impact of sport on social development has been largely debated. Sport is a context that provides participants with the opportunity to control themselves, resolve conflicts, and learn to work with others (Boardley & Kavussanu, 2011). An extensive literature records the impacts of sport and physical activity in improving character since the early 1900s. According to one point of perspectives, sport provides an effective tool through personal and social development and can have a positive influence on young people (Morris, Sallybanks, Willis & Makkai, 2003). Many coaches, sport enthusiasts, and advocates for sport firmly convinced that sport can build positive character values like honesty, responsibility, fairness, and respect, and they suppose them to be one of the ultimate objectives of physical participation (Doty, 2006).
Previous studies have provided evidences in which more intense sport participation such as elite sport increase diverse socio-psychological development. Different levels of sport participation show different social behaviors between elite sport participants and recreational sport participant (Côté & Fraser-Thomas, 2007). Spink (1992) found that both individual attractions to a group task and social integration of a group were different significantly between the elite and the recreational in volleyball, and that cohesiveness was positively related to the elite volleyball teams but not for recreational volleyball teams. Serrao, Martens, Martin and Rocha (2008) found that elite sport participants in collegiate level had higher competitiveness than recreational sport participants did. Also, Šmela, Pačesová, Kraček, and Hájovský (2017) reported that there were significant differences between elite athletes and recreational sport participants in performance motives, anxiety inhibiting performance, and anxiety supporting performance. Nonetheless, it is hard to find an evidence that different participation intensities are associated with prosocial and antisocial behaviors.
Much literature in sport have focused on the negative effect of social behaviors. Sport attracts everyone to do some illegal acts such as rule-breaking and deception (Boardley & Kavussanu, 2011). Sage (1998) revealed that sport did not build character in general, team sport athletes come out more negatively influence by the competitive experience compared to individual athletes. The social nature of sport providing positive educational opportunities, in the meantime quite often leads to the possibility of cheating, lying, bullying and hurting other participants, leading to antisocial behavior (Kavussanu, Seal, & Phillips, 2006). Kroll and Petersen (1965) presented evidence indicates that such principles were less, rather than more, commonly endorsed by regular sport participants.
Generally, social behavior includes prosocial and antisocial behaviors. Prosocial behavior required some sacrifices for the actor and should be done without any external reward (Underwood & Moore, 1982). Antisocial behaviors are the action that harms or lack consideration for the happiness of others (Horowitz, 1987). It has also been defined as any type of conduct that violates the basic rights of another person (Calkins & Keane, 2009) and any behavior that is considered to be disruptive to others in society.
The previous research of prosocial and antisocial behavior have not been sufficient. In the late twentieth century, researchers focus on Rest’s four-component model of social behavior action: awareness, judgment, commitment and action (Rest, 1984), but neglected social behavior reasoning, athlete’s attitudes and orientations, and the judgment of the legality of harmful activities, and so on (Vallerand, Brière, Blanchard, & Provencher, 1997). Bandura (1999) proposed that social behavior had two aspects: proactive and inhibitive. According to the different stages of human growth, Haan (1978) divided the social behavior theories into five levels: assimilative phase (level 1 and 2), accommodative phase (level 3 and 4), equilibration phase (level 5).
The term prosocial behavior has always been associated with developmental characteristics in children (Eisenberg & Mussen, 1989), the literature on the topic has occurred to include adult behaviors since recently as well. As the part of sport studies have primarily considered the inhibitive morality by a survey that has anti-sociality effects on others (Kavussanu, 2008). Studies of the effects of sport competition have revealed that it reduces prosocial tendencies, such as helping and sharing (Barnett & Bryan, 1974; McGuire & Thomas, 1975) and increases antisocial tendencies (Berkowitz, 1972) and all these effects are exacerbated by losing (Kleiber & Roberts, 1981). Al-Yaaribi and Kavussanu (2018) found that prosocial behavior was positively related to effort, commitment and perceived performance in a team sport.
In the studies of sport, researchers have explored different social behaviors. Most sport researches have focused on the related concepts of prosocial and antisocial behavior (Graupensperger, Jensen, & Evans, 2018; Shields, Funk, & Bredemeier, 2018). Sage, Kavussanu, and Duda (2006) utilized prosocial and antisocial behaviors and the dimensions of social behavior. At the same time, Sage and Dual (2006) also addressed some problems of social behavior in sport by using the Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior in Sport Scale (PABSS) which is developed by Kavussanu and Boardley (2009). The PABSS is 20 items and includes 4 factors: individual prosocial and antisocial behavior, directed at either teammates or opponents (Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009). However, most of the participants in the studies were team sport players, and there was a lack of individual sports cases (Funk, 2017). Moreover, scant literature identified the factors constructing prosocial and antisocial behaviors.
Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to find prosocial and antisocial behaviors using an existing social behavior questionnaire and to examine the effect of sport on prosocial and antisocial behaviors. To fulfill the purpose of the study, a research question was set for this study if there were mean differences on social behavior factors between elite sport participation group and recreational sport participation group.

Method

Participants

The present study collected the sample of 700 from 10/02/2017 to 10/25/2017 using the convenience sampling method in Connecticut, Massachusetts, USA. Prior to the survey, the purpose and the procedure of the study were fully explained to the survey participants who granted their permissions to the study. Out of the 700 samples, 107 did not properly answer, did not complete their answers, or refused to answer. Given that, 589 answers were utilized for this study. According to Comrey and Lee (1992), sample sizes of 200,300,500, and 1,000 are considered as enough, good, very good, and excellent, respectively. So, 589 should be sufficient enough for this study. Given the total population over 100,000, the sampling error for the sample of 589 was calculated to ±4% within 95% of confidence level.
The majority participating in soccer (18.5%) followed by basketball (13.9%). 392 (66.6%) experienced elite level sports, and 197 (33.4%) did recreational sports. Especially, the majority responded their best competent participation level was intercollegiate varsity (33.3%). The demographic information of the survey participants is as followed (Table 1).
Table 1.

Demographic information of the sample

f %
Gender
Female 264 44.8
Male 325 55.2
Sport Participation
Soccer 109 18.5
Basketball 82 13.9
Track and Field 55 9.3
Lacrosse 43 7.3
Softball 40 6.8
Volleyball 35 5.9
Baseball 35 5.9
Football 28 4.8
Icehockey 25 4.2
Tennis 21 3.6
Swimming 10 1.7
Others 106 18.0
Competent Participation Level
Elite 392 66.6
National or Professional 25 4.2
Intercollegiate Varsity 196 33.3
Highschool Varsity 171 29.0
Recreational 197 33.4
City / Community League 47 8.0
Intramural League 80 13.6
School Recreation 47 8.0
Others 23 3.9
M SD
Age 24.2 1.66

Instrument and Analysis

The survey questionnaire was developed by Yiannakis and Kane (2004) to verify different social behaviors of sport participants in either recreational or elite level. The survey originally consists of 31 items in 5-point scale format; 1 is ‘most like me’ and 5 is ‘least like me.’ This study adapted the survey questionnaire with minimal modification.
Validity and reliability of the measurement were assessed (Table 2). Face validity was established from a committee panel of 3 professors for the topic area. Using factor analysis, construct validity was verified. The KMO was .851 which was considered adequate, and the Barlett’s test indicated also adequate enough, χ2​(351)=4574.56, p<.001. Given that, 27 items constructing 7 factors were utilized for the survey includeing 5 prosocial behavior factors (emotional support, leadership, social facilitation, arbitration, and group cohesiveness), and 2 antisocial behavior factors (aggressiveness and inner group conflict). These factors seem properly structured based upon the followings; the eigenvalue greater than 1 using the Kaiser’s rule, each factor constructed with at least 3 items (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007; Zwick & Velicer, 1986); and accumulated percentage approximately ranged between 50 to 60 (Hair, Anderson, Tatham, & Black, 1995). The internal consistency α coefficients of the factors ranged from .553 to .786, which seem leniently acceptable.
Table 2.

Validity and reliability result

Factor Item NO. Factor
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Emotional support Q29 .787 .142 -.111 -.033 .009 .008 -.009
Q9 .710 .212 .005 .019 .204 -.012 .022
Q28 .611 .216 -.236 .052 .161 .113 .089
Q16 .575 .092 -.203 .138 .253 .041 .319
Q3 .525 .017 -.391 .138 .139 -.042 .244
Leadership Q30 .370 .635 -.128 .118 -.017 .112 .082
Q17 .091 .629 -.077 .290 .104 .192 .194
Q14 .266 .601 .098 .208 .285 -.034 .165
Q20 .278 .596 -.032 .169 .280 .113 .086
Q21 -.076 .507 .032 .070 .162 .498 .151
Aggressiveness Q19 -.087 -.139 .752 -.008 .000 .144 .146
Q11 -.203 .078 .738 .021 .150 .087 -.113
Q12 -.026 -.038 .655 .383 -.060 .087 -.048
Q6 -.089 .177 .403 .059 -.014 .369 .375
Social Facilitation Q15 .039 .297 .073 .733 .030 .112 .190
Q24 -.044 -.093 .191 .700 .151 .071 .151
Q7 .103 .236 -.101 .659 .025 .236 .121
Arbitration Q18 .055 -.113 -.146 -.069 .735 .101 .066
Q8 -.072 .270 .233 .296 .572 .138 -.101
Q2 .355 .209 -.038 .237 .507 .037 -.150
Q10 .261 .227 .162 .023 .483 -.128 .166
Innergroup Conflict Q13 .048 .007 -.039 .168 .137 .732 .016
Q31 .073 -.009 .286 .078 -.135 .682 -.058
Q1 -.052 .386 .261 .115 .352 .400 .148
Group Cohesiveness Q26 -.020 .230 -.232 .016 .046 .195 .632
Q27 -.016 .129 .159 .224 -.085 -.086 .578
Q5 .348 -.246 .018 .176 .127 -.045 .544
Coefficient .786 .772 .683 .681 .556 .553 .574
Eigenvalue 5.71 3.47 1.531 1.41 1.121 1.033 1.001
% Dispersion 21.15 12.852 5.671 5.221 4.15 3.826 3.708
% Accumulate 21.15 34.002 39.673 44.894 49.044 52.871 56.579
KMO = .851 χ2 (351) = 4574.56 p <.001
To analyze the collected data, the present study employed descriptive analyses, a factor analysis, and a one-way MANOVA to verify group differences with the statistical significance of .05 (α=.05) using the SPSS 23.0.

Results

Table 3 shows the mean difference between the elite sport participant and the recreational sport participant on each dependent variable. On most of the social behavior factors, the mean scores of the recreational sport participant group were higher than those of the elite sport participant group, except for the arbitration and aggressiveness.
Table 3.

Descriptive analysis result

DV Group n M SD
Prosocial
Behavior
Leadership Elite Sport 392 2.26 .710
Recreational Sport 197 2.62 .901
Social
Facilitation
Elite Sport 392 2.50 .888
Recreational Sport 197 2.85 .943
Group
Cohesiveness
Elite Sport 392 2.41 .777
Recreational Sport 197 2.66 .911
Emotional
Support
Elite Sport 392 2.25 .717
Recreational Sport 197 2.46 .857
Arbitration Elite Sport 392 2.67 .745
Recreational Sport 197 2.65 .848
Antisocial
Behavior
Aggressiveness Elite Sport 392 3.69 .836
Recreational Sport 197 3.67 .958
Innergroup
Conflict
Elite Sport 392 2.52 .794
Recreational Sport 197 2.77 .894
The present study conducted a one-way MANOVA as followed. Prior to the main test, the multivariate test for homogeneity of dispersion matrices, Box’s Test was examined, and it was significant, which rejected the homogeneity assumption, Box’s M=65.47, F(28, 569673.63)=2.304, p<.001. Given that, Levene’s Test for the homogeneity of variance was also examined, and the results were also significant except for social facilitation and innergroup conflict (Table 4). However, if the sample size is bigger than 30, MANOVA is robust against the violation of homogeneity of variance-​covariance matrices assumption, Box’s Test, and a stricter α level than .05 should also be considered for univariate F test such as .001 to compensate a significant Levene’s Test result (Allen & Bennett, 2008).
Table 4.

Univariate tests of homogeneity

DV F(1,587) p
Leadership 14.667 <.001
Social Facilitation .765 .382*
Group Cohesiveness 5.443 .020
Emotional Support 12.750 <.001
Arbitration 7.383 .007
Aggressiveness 10.338 .001
Innergroup Conflict 2.821 .094*

* indicates the homogeneity of variance assumption met at α = .05.

The MANOVA result shows a significant difference across the two groups on the set of dependent variables (Table 5). Hotelling’s T-Square indicated a statistical significance between the elite sport participation group and the recreational sport participation group, Hotelling’s T2=.094, F(7, 581)=7.833, p<.001. 8.6% of the generalized variance for the set of DVs was explained by the group difference.
Table 5.

MANOVA result between the elite sport group and the recreational sport group

DV F p η2
MANOVA
DV set 7.833 <.001* .086
ANOVAs (Follow-ups)
Prosocial
Behavior
Leadership 28.591 <.001** .046
Social Facilitation 19.086 <.001** .031
Group Cohesiveness 12.403 <.001** .021
Emotional Support 9.732 .002 .016
Arbitration .121 .728 <.001
Antisocial
Behavior
Aggressiveness .068 .794 <.001
Innergroup Conflict 11.314 .001 .019

* indicates the mean difference statistically significant at α = .05.

** indicates the mean difference statistically significant at α = .001, using a corrected α due to the violation of the homogeneity of variance.

ANOVAs on dependent variables were conducted as follow-up tests to the MANOVA (Table 5). Because of the significant result of the Levene’s Test as explained the above, each ANOVA was tested at .001 level. ANOVAs were significant on the leadership, F(1,587)=28.591, p<.001, η2=.046; the social facilitation, F(1, 587)=19.086, p<.001, η2=.031; and, the group cohesiveness, F(1,587)=12.403, p<.001, η2=.021. However, other ANOVAs were nonsignificant on the emotional support, F(1,587)=9.732, p=.002, η2=.016; the arbitration, F(1,587)=.121, p=.728, η2<.001; the aggressiveness, F(1,587)=.068, p=.794, η2<.001; and the innergroup conflict, F(1,587)=11.314, p=.001, η2=.019.
In sum, the social behavior of the elite sport participant group and that of the recreational sport participant group was statistically different. Especially, the recreational group scored higher than the elite group on 3 of the prosocial behavior factors including the leadership, the social facilitation, and the group cohesiveness. However, there was no difference on the antisocial behaviors.

Discussion and Conclusion

Factors of social behavior in sport

Social behavior in sports has gained increased attention in recent years. Social behavior is largely divided into prosocial behavior dimension and antisocial behavior dimension (Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009). However, both behavior dimensions have not been specifically defined by more detail factors or forming a multidimensional hierarchical construct in literature. Given that, the present study attempted to identify specific factors constructing prosocial and antisocial behaviors using a previously developed questionnaire. An exploratory factor analysis could determine the factorial structure of prosocial behavior and antisocial behavior. The analysis found 5 factors of prosocial behaviors (emotional support, leadership, social facilitation, arbitration, and group cohesiveness) and 2 factors of antisocial behaviors (aggressiveness, and innergroup conflict) in sport settings.
Those seven factors found from the exploratory factor analysis were supported by the literature. In this study, emotional support is linked to items such as encouragement and comforting. Emotional support is an important motivating factor for prosocial behavior (Lockwood, Seara-Cardoso, & Viding, 2014), and it consists of a specific communicative behavior developed by one party with the intention to help others cope effectively with emotional distress (Greene & Burleson, 2003). Labile (2007) demonstrated that the emotional skills that teenagers learn play an important role in fostering prosocial behavior in the context of close attachment relationships.
Leadership consists of items such as reliability and responsibility. Leadership is led to prosocial behaviors (George & Bettenhausen, 1990), and is to draw the best ideas and management democratically and collegially (Summerfield, 2014).
Social facilitation is understood as outstanding performance in the present of others in this study. Social facilitation is for people to perform better on some tasks in the presence of others (Wu, Huang, Shadiev, & Ho, 2015). Baldassarri and Grossman (2013) pointed out that the social facilitation as a function of formal roles could add prosocial behavior.
In this study, arbitration refers not to humiliate the opposition, to point out what members do wrong, and to help them by demonstrating the correct skills. Arbitration, another prosocial behavior, is a useful way of resolving arguments between people by helping them to agree to a solution (Pedersen, 2000). Stevenson (1991) explained that arbitration is a skill to improve prosocial behavior.
Group cohesiveness means to involve actively in team activities. Group cohesiveness, an increasing prosocial behavior (Hogg & Turner, 1987), is often accompanied by feelings of solidarity, harmony, and commitment in its members (Dion, 2000). Consequently, those factors of the prosocial behavior are consistent with the literature.
Aggression is restricted to the type of behavioral aggression described. The aggressiveness is one of the most special characters of antisocial behavior and is originated from Children’s Social Behavior Scale (Crick, 1996).
Innergroup conflict occurs in the context of tasks and relationships and includes cognitive, emotional, and behavioral components (Paradis, Carron, & Martin, 2014). Studying innergroup conflict is important in terms of antisocial behavior because a number of previous research indicates innergroup conflict associated with identity problems, self-harm behavior, lower interpersonal functioning, and other antisocial behaviors (Doran, 2014). Thus, the above two factors are useful to measure antisocial behavior.

Sport participants and social behavior

The result indicated that there were differences in social behaviors between the elite sport participant and the recreational sport participant. This finding was compared with the previous literature. Jamieson and Ross (2007) found that sports greatly promoted socialization, and reduced the rate of aggression. Chen, Snyder, and Magner (2010) indicated that sports participation strengthened social behaviors and relations. Nonetheless, Butt, Mansoor, Akhar, Saeed, and Adnan (2016) revealed different social behaviors between athletes and regular students, and the regular students were more social than the athletes were.
Diametrically opposed to the above, Sage (1998) stated that sports did not build character or social behavior in general. Bredemeier, Shields, and Shields (1986) found that participation in sports did not provide a markedly positive effect, but had a negative influence. Shields and Bredemeier (2001) insisted that sports building characters were just a cultural adage, but actually, sports did not act such a thing. Given the above previous studies, the literature has not been consistent with the finding of the present study, which there were differences on social behaviors between the elite sport participant and the recreational sport participant.

Sport participation and prosocial behavior

The present study found that prosocial behavior of the elite sport participant and that of recreational sport participant have marked differences. Especially, leadership, social facilitation, and group cohesion were identified as 3 specific prosocial behaviors on which the recreational sport participants were higher than the elite sport participants. That was supported by the existing literature in detail as followed.
Firstly, a significant difference in leadership was found between the elite sport participant and the recreational sport participant, and the recreational sport participant showed higher in leadership than the elite sport participant. This finding drew disparity with the previous literature regarding the athlete demonstrating greater leadership ability than non-athletes (Dobosz & Beaty, 1999). Also, Extejt and Smith (2009) found no association between the number of sport participation and the level of any particular leadership skill. Yukelson, Weinberg, Richardson, and Jackson (1983) examined the collegiate athletes not having higher leadership compared to other college students. Consequently, intensive or serious sport participation does not guarantee any meaningful association with high leadership.
Secondly, the recreational sport participant was higher on social facilitation than the elite sport participant that was similar to Cottrell (1968). However, this result indicated oppositely to the result of no difference on social facilitation between expert and novice player (Forgas, Brennan, Howe, Kane, & Sweet, 1980). Uziel (2007) pointed out that participation and mastery levels could differentiate the social facilitation. Nonetheless, along with Cottrell (1968), the present study confirmed that serious participation in sport did not promote social facilitation.
Thirdly, the recreational sport participant showed higher group cohesion level than the elite sport participant. This result is backed up by previous findings such as Lowther and Lane (2002). Their study demonstrated that cohesion did not reliably differentiate between high-level athletes and others. In consequence, serious sport participation did not guarantee high group cohesion, and even recreational participation could increase group cohesion greater than elite sport participation.
According to the result of the present study, the recreational sport participants marked higher scores on prosocial behaviors than the elite sport participants did. It might be caused by pursuits to different value orientations between the groups. The elite sport is always to pursue outstanding performance with competitive spirit. The participants are strictly requested to win. They devote themselves intensely in practice and sacrifice something in the process for win in the competition (Chelladurai, 2007). For another, recreational sport participants are to pursue pleasure mostly. Chatzisarantis and Hagger (2007) have noted that recreational sport participants showed a preference for intrinsic life aspirations compared with elite sport participant and showed up higher psychological well-being. The above orientation might cause the difference in some prosocial difference factors between the groups of the present study.

Sport participation and antisocial behavior

The present study also found that the elite sport participant and the recreational sport participant have no significant difference on antisocial behaviors including aggressiveness and innergroup conflict. The result stood in contrast to Tenenbaum, Singer, Stewart, and Duda (1997) in their International Social Survey Programme Position Statement on Violence inside sport in which elite athletes tended to show reduced violence. Laker (2000), Estes (2003), Clarke (2012), and Collinson, Judege, Stanley, and Wilson (2015) all found similar results to Tenenbaum, et al. (1997).
On the contrary, Lemieux, McKelvie and Stout (2002), Rhea and Lantz (2004), and Bruner, Boardley, and Côté (2014) similarly found that athletes did not show antisocial behaviors differently compared to non-athletes. It thus should be noted that antisocial behavior of different level of sport participants is no different.

Conclusion

In sum, the present study founds as the followings. First, utilizing an existing questionnaire, this study identified diverse facets of prosocial and antisocial behaviors in sport, which could be comparable to the previous studies. There were 5 prosocial behaviors (emotional support, leadership, social facilitation, arbitration, and group cohesiveness) and 2 antisocial behaviors (aggressiveness, and innergroup conflict) in sport settings. Second, an elite sport participation that was more intensive and serious participation in sport did not increase social behavior; furthermore, less intensive participation forms could provide better social behavior. Third, the recreational sport participation and the elite sport participation differ on prosocial behavior. Especially in leadership, social facilitation, and group cohesion, the recreational sport showed higher than the elite sport. Fourth, the elite sport participant and the recreational sport participant have no significant difference on antisocial behaviors including aggressiveness and innergroup conflict.
Given the above, overly serious participations do not guarantee social behaviors positively. According to this, those organizations that want to promote social development through sports, then they should pay more attention to the promotion of the recreational sport. Those who claim that sport brings social instability also should be noted that antisocial behavior of different level of sport participants is no different. Generally, however, this research should be regarded as preliminary. Consideration of other team sports or individual sports, various age or gender, and different regions, and more sophisticated longitudinal research designs will all be needed to adequately determine the role of sport in social development. It would be interesting for future research to find out these possibilities.

References

1. . Al-Yaaribi, A., et al, & Kavussanu, M. (2018). Consequences of prosocial and antisocial behaviors in adolescent male soccer players: The moderating role of motivational climate. Psychology of Sport and Exercise , 37, 91-99. DOI: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.04.005.
crossref
2. . Allen, P., et al, Bennett, K. (2008, SPSS for the Health & Behavioural Sciences . South Melbourne Thompson.
3. Baldassarri, D., et al, & Grossman, G. (2013). The effect of group attachment and social position on prosocial behavior. Evidence from lab-in-the-field experiments. PLOS one , 8(3), e58750 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058750.
crossref
4. Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review , 3(3), 193-209. DOI: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0303_3.
crossref pdf
5. . Barnett, M. A., et al, & Bryan, J. H. (1974). Effects of competition with outcome feedback on children’s helping behavior. Developmental Psychology , 10(6), 838 DOI: 10.1037/h0037255.
crossref
6. . Berkowitz, L. (1972, Social norms feelings, and other factors affecting helping and altruism. In. In: Berkowitz L. (Ed.). In: Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (pp. 63. -108). New York. Academic Press.
7. Boardley, I. D., et al, & Kavussanu, M. (2011). Moral disengagement in sport. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology , 4(2), 93-108. DOI: 10.1080/1750984x.2011.570361.
crossref
8. Bredemeier, B. J., Shields, D. L., et al, & Shields, D. L. (1986). Moral growth among athletes and nonathletes: A comparative analysis. The Journal of Genetic Psychology , 147(1), 7-18. DOI: 10.1080/00221325.1986.9914475.
crossref
9. Bruner, M. W., Boardley, I. D., et al, & Côté, J. (2014). Social identity and prosocial and antisocial behavior in youth sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise , 15(1), 56-64. DOI: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.09.003.
crossref
10. Butt, Z. I., Rashid, K., Mansoor, N., Akhtar, T., Saeed, N., et al, & Adnan, M. A. J. (2016). Effects of sports on social behavior of university students. Science International , 28(2), 1525-1529.
11. Calkins, S. D., et al, & Keane, S. P. (2009). Developmental origins of early antisocial behavior. Development and Psychopathology , 21(4), 1095-1109. DOI: 10.1017/s095457940999006x.
crossref
12. Chatzisarantis, N. L., et al, & Hagger, M. S. (2007). The moral worth of sport reconsidered: Contributions of recreational sport and competitive sport to life aspirations and psychological well-being. Journal of Sports Sciences , 25(9), 1047-1056. DOI: 10.1080/02640410600959954.
crossref
13. Chelladurai, P. (2007). Leadership in sports. Handbook of Sport Psychology , 3, 113-135.
crossref pdf
14. Chen, S., Snyder, S., et al, & Magner, M. (2010). The effects of sport participation on student-athletes’ and non-​athlete students’ social life and identity. Journal of Issues in Intercollegiate Athletics , 3(1), 176-193.
15. Crick, N. R. (1996). The role of overt aggression, relational aggression, and prosocial behavior in the prediction of children’s future social adjustment. Child Development , 67(5), 2317-2327. DOI: 10.2307/1131625.
crossref
16. Clarke, GC. (2012). Why do youth step out of sport and into court? A Narrative-Based Exploration, Masters Thesis. University of Waikato, Department of Sport and Leisure,
17. Collinson, L., Judge, L., Stanley, J., et al, & Wilson, N. (2015). Portrayal of violence, weapons, antisocial behaviour and alcohol: Study of televised music videos in New Zealand. New Zealand Medical Journal , 128​(1410), 84-86.
18. Comrey, A. L., et al, Lee, H. B. (1992, A first course in factor analysis . 2nd edition. Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum.
19. . Cottrell, N. B., Wack, D. L., Sekerak, G. J., et al, & Rittle, R. H. (1968). Social facilitation of dominant responses by the presence of an audience and the mere presence of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 9(3), 245 DOI: 10.1037/h0025902.
crossref
20. Côté, J., et al, Fraser-Thomas, J. (2007, Youth involvement in sport. In. In: Crocker P. (Ed.). In: Sport psychology: A Canadian perspective (pp. 270. -298). Toronto. Pearson.
21. . Dion, K. L. (2000). Group cohesion: From “field of forces” to multidimensional construct. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice , 4(1), 7 DOI: 10.1037/1089-2699.4.1.7.
crossref
22. . Dobosz, R. P., et al, & Beaty, L. A. (1999). The relationship between athletic participation and high school students' leadership ability. Adolescence, 34(133), 215
pmid
23. . Doran, B. L. 2014; Social identity and social learning factors as predictors of intergroup and intragroup social aggressiveness in college sororities Order No. 3641977. Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (1629473590). Retrieved from https://search.proquest.com/docview/1629473590?accountid=17382.
24. . Doty, J. (2006). Sports build character?! Journal of College and Character , 7(3), DOI: 10.2202/1940-1639.1529.
crossref
25. Eisenberg, N., et al, Mussen, P. H. (1989, The roots of prosocial behavior in children . New York, NY Cambridge University Press. DOI: 10.1017/cbo9780511571121.
26. Estes, S. (2003, Physical Education and Educational Sport. The future of physical education: Building a new pedagogy . London Routledge.
27. Extejt, M. M., et al, & Smith, J. E. (2009). Leadership development through sports team participation. Journal of Leadership Education , 8(2), 224-237. DOI: 10.12806/v8/i2/rf7.
crossref
28. Forgas, J. P., Brennan, G., Howe, S., Kane, J. F., et al, & Sweet, S. (1980). Audience effects on squash players’ performance. The Journal of Social Psychology , 111​(1), 41-47. DOI: 10.1080/00224545.1980.9924271.
crossref
29. . Funk, C. D. 2017; Elements of moral functioning in sport and school Order No. 10685601. Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (1984981484). Retrieved from https://search.proquest.com/docview/1984981484?accountid=17382.
30. . George, J. M., et al, & Bettenhausen, K. (1990). Understanding prosocial behavior, sales performance, and turnover: A group-level analysis in a service context. Journal of Applied Psychology , 75(6), 698 DOI: 10.1037//0021-9010.75.6.698.
crossref
31. . Graupensperger, S. A., Jensen, C. J., et al, & Evans, M. B. (2018). A meta-analytic review of studies using the Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior in Sport Scale: Associations among intergroup moral behaviors. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology , 7(2), 186 DOI: 10.1037/spy0000121.
crossref
32. . Greene, J. O., et al, Burleson, B. R. (2003, Handbook of communication and social interaction skills . Mahwah, NJ Erlbaum. DOI: 10.4324/9781410607133.
33. . Haan, N. (1978). Two moralities in action contexts: Relationships to thought, ego regulation, and development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 36(3), 286 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.36.3.286.
crossref
34. Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., et al, Black, W. C. (1995, Multivariate data analysis: with readings . 4th ed.Prentice-Hall, Inc.
35. Hogg, M. A., et al, & Turner, J. C. (1987). Intergroup behaviour, self‐stereotyping and the salience of social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology , 26(4), 325-340. DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1987.tb00795.x.
crossref
36. . Horowitz, F. D. (1987). The Developing Person Through Childhood and Adolescence. Psyccritiques , 32(2), 193
37. . Jamieson, L. M., et al, & Ross, C. M. (2007). Research Update: Using Recreation to Curb Extremism-An effective means for peace building exists in your community. Parks & Recreation , 42(2), 26
38. Kavussanu, M. (2008). Moral behaviour in sport: A critical review of the literature. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology , 1(2), 124-138. DOI: 10.1080/17509840802277417.
crossref
39. Kavussanu, M., et al, & Boardley, I. D. (2009). The prosocial and antisocial behavior in sport scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology , 31(1), 97-117. DOI: 10.1123/jsep.31.1.97.
crossref
40. Kavussanu, M., Seal, A. R., et al, & Phillips, D. R. (2006). Observed Prosocial and Antisocial Behaviors in Male Soccer Teams: Age Differences across Adolescence and the Role of Motivational Variables. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology , 18(4), 326-​344. DOI: 10.1080/10413200600944108.
crossref
41. Kleiber, D. A., et al, & Roberts, G. G. (1981). The effects of sport experience in the development of social character: An exploratory investigation. Journal of Sport Psychology , 3(2), 114-122. DOI: 10.1123/jsp.3.2.114.
crossref
42. Kroll, W., et al, & Petersen, K. H. (1965). Study of values test and collegiate football teams. Research Quarterly. American Association for Health, Physical Education and Recreation , 36(4), 441-447. DOI: 10.1080/10671188.1965.10614717.
crossref
43. Laible, D. (2007). Attachment with parents and peers in late adolescence: Links with emotional competence and social behavior. Personality and Individual Differences , 43(5), 1185-1197. DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.010.
crossref
44. Laker, A. (2000, Beyound the Boundaries of Physical Education . London: New York Routledge Falmer. DOI: 10.4324/9780203135792.
45. Lemieux, P., McKelvie, S. J., et al, & Stout, D. (2002). Self-​reported hostile aggression in contact athletes, no contact athletes and non-athletes. Athletic Insight , 4(3), 42-56.
46. . Lockwood, P. L., Seara-Cardoso, A., et al, & Viding, E. (2014). Emotion regulation moderates the association between empathy and prosocial behavior. PLOS one , 9(5), e96555 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096555.
crossref
47. Lowther, J., et al, & Lane, A. (2002). Relationships between mood, cohesion and satisfaction with performance among soccer players. Athletic Insight , 4(3), 57-69.
48. Morris, L., Sallybanks, J., Willis, K., et al, & Makkai, T. (2003). Sport, physical activity and anti-social behaviour in youth. Trends and Issues in Crime and Criminal Justice , 249, 1-6.
49. . McGuire, J. M., et al, & Thomas, M. H. (1975). Effects of sex, competence, and competition on sharing behavior in children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 32(3), 490 DOI: 10.1037/h0077091.
crossref
50. Paradis, K. F., Carron, A. V., et al, & Martin, L. J. (2014). Athlete perceptions of intra-group conflict in sport teams. Sport & Exercise Psychology Review , 10(3), 4-18.
crossref
51. . Pedersen, P. (2000, A handbook for developing multicultural awareness . 3rd ed.Alexandria, VA American Counseling Association.
52. Rest, J. R. (1984). Research on moral development: Implications for training counseling psychologists. The Counseling Psychologist , 12(3), 19-29. DOI: 10.1177/0011000084123003.
crossref pdf
53. Rhea, D. J., et al, & Lantz, C. D. (2004). Violent, delinquent, and aggressive behaviors of rural high school athletes and non-athletes. Physical Educator , 61(4), 170-176.
54. Sage. (1998). Does sport affect character development in athletes? Journal of Physical Education, Recreation & Dance, 69(1), 15-18..Sage. (1998). Does sport affect character development in athletes? Journal of Physical Education, Recreation & Dance , 69(1), 15-18.
crossref
55. Sage, Kavussanu, M., et al, & Duda, J. (2006). Goal orientations and moral identity as predictors of prosocial and antisocial functioning in male association football players. Journal of Sports Sciences , 24(05), 455-​466. DOI: 10.1080/02640410500244531.
crossref
56. Serrao, H. F., Martens, M. P., Martin, J. L., et al, & Rocha, T. L. (2008). Competitiveness and alcohol use among recreational and elite collegiate athletes. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology , 2(3), 205-215. DOI: 10.1123/jcsp.2.3.205.
crossref
57. . Shields, D. L., et al, & Bredemeier, B. (2001). Moral development and behavior in sport. Handbook of Sport Psychology , 2, 585-603.
58. Shields, D. L., Funk, C. D., et al, & Bredemeier, B. L. (2018). Relationships among moral and contesting variables and prosocial and antisocial behavior in sport. Journal of Moral Education , 47(1), 17-33. DOI: 10.1080/03057240.2017.1350149.
crossref
59. Šmela, P., Pačesová, P., Kraček, S., et al, & Hájovský, D. (2017). Performance Motivation of Elite Athletes, Recreational Athletes and Non-Athletes. Acta Facultatis Educationis Physicae Universitatis Comenianae , 57(2), 125-133. DOI: 10.1515/afepuc-2017-0012.
crossref
60. Spink, K. S. (1992). Group cohesion and starting status in successful and less successful elite volleyball teams. Journal of Sports Sciences , 10(4), 379-388. DOI: 10.1080/02640419208729934.
crossref
61. . Stevenson, H. W. (1991). The development of prosocial behavior in large-scale collective societies: China and Japan. Cooperation and prosocial behaviour , 89-105.
62. Summerfield, M. R. (2014). Leadership: A simple definition. American Journal of Health-System Pharmacy , 71(3), 251-253. DOI: 10.2146/ajhp130435.
crossref
63. . Tabachnick, B. G., et al, Fidell, L. S. (2007, Using multivariate statistics . 5th. Needham Height, MA Allyn & Bacon.
64. Tenenbaum, G., Singer, R. N., Stewart, E., et al, & Duda, J. (1997). Aggression and violence in sport: An ISSP position stand. The Sport Psychologist , 11(1), 1-7. DOI: 10.1123/tsp.11.1.1.
crossref
65. . Underwood, B., et al, & Moore, B. (1982). Perspective-taking and altruism. Psychological Bulletin , 91(1), 143 DOI: 10.1037//0033-2909.91.1.143.
crossref
66. United Nations. (2008). The Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs). New York: United Nations.United Nations. (2008, The Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs) . New York United Nations.
67. Uziel, L. (2007). Individual differences in the social facilitation effect: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Personality , 41(3), 579-601. DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.06.008.
crossref
68. Vallerand, R. J., Brière, N. M., Blanchard, C., et al, & Provencher, P. (1997). Development and validation of the multidimensional sportspersonship orientations scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology , 19(2), 197-206. DOI: 10.1123/jsep.19.2.197.
crossref
69. . Wu, T.-T., Huang, Y.-M., Shadiev, R., et al, & Ho, H.-L. (2015). Does the social facilitation effect occur in web-based learning environment? International Journal of Technology and Engineering Education , 12(2), 1
70. . Yiannakis, , et al, Kane, (2004, Behavior in sport v.3.04. Unpublished manuscript. University of Connecticut.
71. . Yukelson, D., Weinberg, R., Richardson, P., et al, & Jackson, A. (1983). Interpersonal attraction and leadership within collegiate sport teams. Journal of Sport Behavior , 6(1), 28
72. Zwick, W. R., et al, & Velicer, W. F. (1986). Comparison of five rules for determining the number of components to retain. Endocrinology , 76(76), 646-656.
crossref
TOOLS
METRICS Graph View
  • 1 Crossref
  •  0 Scopus
  • 1,191 View
  • 31 Download
Related articles


ABOUT
BROWSE ARTICLES
EDITORIAL POLICY
FOR CONTRIBUTORS
Editorial Office
Korea Institute of Sport Science, 727 Hwarang-lo, Nowon-gu, Seoul 01794, Korea                         Privacy Policy
Tel: +82-2-970-9570    Fax: +82-2-970-9651    E-mail: publ@kspo.or.kr                

Copyright © 2024 by Korea Institute of Sport Science.

Close layer
prev next